Evidence for ... Inhibitory Control Abilities ...

Human Brain Mapping

Kärgel, Christian, Massau Claudia, Weiss Simone, Walter Martin, Borchardt Viola, Krueger Tillman H. C., et al.; Oct 21 2016
Volume38
Issue2
Type of WorkResearch Report

Quotes and summaries from

Evidence for Superior Neurobiological and Behavioral Inhibitory Control Abilities in Non-Offending as Compared to Offending Pedophiles

Abstract

Neurobehavioral models of pedophilia and child sexual offending suggest a pattern of temporal and in particular prefrontal disturbances leading to inappropriate behavioral control and subsequently an increased propensity to sexually offend against children. However, clear empirical evidence for such mechanisms is still missing. Using a go/nogo paradigm in combination with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) we compared behavioral performance and neural response patterns among three groups of men matched for age and IQ:

  • pedophiles with (N=40) and
  • without (N=37) a history of hands-on sexual offences against children as well as
  • healthy non-offending controls (N=40).

As compared to offending pedophiles, non-offending pedophiles exhibited superior inhibitory control as reflected by significantly lower rate of commission errors. Group-by-condition interaction analysis also revealed inhibition-related activation in the left posterior cingulate and the left superior frontal cortex that distinguished between offending and non-offending pedophiles, while no significant differences were found between pedophiles and healthy controls.

Both areas showing distinct activation pattern among pedophiles play a critical role in linking neural networks that relate to effective cognitive functioning. Data therefore suggest that heightened inhibition-related recruitment of these areas as well as decreased amount of commission errors is related to better inhibitory control in pedophiles who successfully avoid committing hands-on sexual offences against children.

Introduction

According to the 10th Edition of the International Statistical Classification System of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD-10) [World Health Organization, 1992], pedophilia is defined as a sexual preference characterized by sexual attraction towards prepubertal or early pubertal children. In popular usage, pedophilia is often equated with child molestation, but clinically both phenomena can be distinguished.

Empirical investigations identified pedophilia to be a major risk factor for committing sexual offenses against children [Seto et al., 2006]. However,

  • individuals with a diagnosis of pedophilia do not necessarily commit child sexual offenses (CSO), just as
  • most of the persons who sexually offend against children do not fulfill diagnostic criteria for pedophilia [Beier et al., 2015].

In this line, previous studies suggested that only 40–50% of the juridical recorded cases of child sexual offenders are committed by pedophiles [Seto, 2008].

[... ... ... A view on the research on pedophilia / pedosexuality and the functioning of their brains ... ... ...]

Differences in the composition of comparison groups regarding both experimental and control groups, mostly small sample sizes, and the usage of deliberate statistical thresholds among previous studies have been identified to be major methodological issues in the discussion of what might explain the contradictory findings. [...]

  • While some studies controlled for the influence of offence status by comparing groups of offending pedophiles with non-sexual offenders [Cantor et al., 2008, 2015; Poeppl et al., 2011],
  • other studies compared (mixed groups of non-offending and) offending pedophiles with non-pedophilic non-offenders [Habermeyer et al., 2013a, 2013b; Schiffer et al., 2007].

Since none of the previous studies differed between offending and non-offending groups of pedophiles, it remains largely unclear to what extent their results are related to a deviant sexual preference, i.e., pedophilia or aberrant behavior, i.e., CSO [*].

  • [* = Child Sexual Offences - see list of abbreviations - Ipce]

Response inhibition, defined as the ability to withhold a pre-potent response, represents a major aspect of executive function [Bari and Robbins, 2013] and has closely been associated to facets of impulsivity or self control, depending on the observed amount of errors and reaction times.

The behavioral and neurobiological aspects of response inhibition have frequently been investigated by using go/nogo paradigms, during which participants are required to respond as quickly as possible to a frequently presented target stimulus and to withhold a pre-potent response to a rarely presented distractor. Although there is evidence that neural activation related to response inhibition is taskdependent [Simmonds et al., 2008], the neural network involved in response inhibition commonly include

  • the supplementary motor area (SMA),
  • premotor cortex,
  • inferior parietal cortex,
  • DLPFC,
  • VLPFC, and
  • the insula [Bari and Robbins, 2013; Chikazoe, 2010].

Poor task performance as well as altered activity within the former network has been confirmed by a variety of studies during response inhibition in populations showing impulsive behavior such as

  • attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder,
  • borderline personality disorder,
  • antisocial personality disorder or
  • drug abusers
    [Cubillo et al., 2010; Sebastian et al., 2014; V€ollm et al., 2004; van Zutphen et al., 2015].

While nonpedophilic child sexual offenders were also found to respond more impulsively [Eastvold, 2010; Schiffer and Vonlaufen, 2011], a recent review of the corresponding literature did not corroborate the hypothesis of poor performance on response inhibition in pedophilic offenders [Joyal et al., 2014].

Other recent studies demonstrated slower processing speed across a range of neuropsychological domains in offending pedophiles [Suchy et al., 2009, 2014] and provided evidence that this deficit may be related to fundamental neurocognitive impairments rather than a deliberate response style.

A comprehensive overview on the neuropsychology of sex offenders including 23 studies can be found in Joyal et al. [2014]. Accordingly, the authors point to the importance to distinguish between specific subgroups of sex offenders, as for instance on the basis of criminological typologies.

Up until now, there is only one study that addressed the neurobiological correlates of response inhibition in eleven pedophilic offenders from an outpatient treatment facility [Habermeyer et al., 2013b]. By utilizing a blocked go/nogo task during fMRI, Habermeyer et al. reported

  • slower reaction time,
  • less accurate visual target discrimination and
  • attenuated deactivation of the left PCC/precuneus and the angular gyrus during the no-go condition

in pedophiles as compared to seven healthy controls.

The finding of inhibition-related alterations in brain areas which are part of the “Default Mode Network” (DMN) [Buckner et al., 2008] seems noteworthy, suggesting that pedophiles may have problems keeping their attention focused during cognitively challenging tasks. However, since the authors only assessed a small and mixed group of offending and non-offending pedophiles (with six committing CSO and five consuming child pornography), the differential effect of sexual age preference and offensive behavior on neural correlates of inhibitory control remained unclear.

By using a letter based go/nogo task in combination with fMRI, the present study aimed at disentangling behavioral and neural characteristics of response inhibition processes associated with pedophilia or CSO.

Although the consumption of child pornography is generally regarded as an offence, here we focused on hands-on sexual contacts with children when referring to the offender status of a participant, which reflects a more severe and direct type of child sexual abuse and is of particular interest in the assessment of response inhibition.

We therefore compared three groups of men:

  • (1) pedophiles with a history of hands-on sexual offending against children (P+CSO),
  • (2) pedophiles who did not engage in hands-on child sexual offending (P-CSO), and
  • (3) healthy controls without any criminal conviction or paraphilia (HC).

Up until now, there is a lack of studies comparing the ability to inhibit pre-potent response tendencies in pedophiles who engaged or not engaged in child sexual offending.

Since only the latter group is characterized by the ability to avoid hands-on child sexual offences, we expected to find a pattern of increased inhibitory control in nonoffending pedophiles as compared to offending pedophiles, as indicated by the number of commission errors.

According to the finding that the VMPFC plays a critical role in inhibitory control [Bari and Robbins, 2013; Chikazoe, 2010] and, moreover, was reported to be altered in offending pedophiles [Kruger and Schiffer, 2011; Mendez and Shapira, 2011; Schiffer et al., 2007; Schiffer and Vonlaufen, 2011], we subsequently hypothesized that P+CSO as compared to both non-offending groups would show different BOLD signal change during response inhibition in related medial prefrontal areas. Similarly, in P+CSO we expected aberrant BOLD activity in medial parietal regions [Habermeyer et al., 2013b; K€argel et al., 2015] when compared to P-CSO and HC.

Methods

Participants

Participants of the present study were assessed within the framework of a German multi-site research project called “Neural Mechanisms Underlying Pedophilia and Sexual Offending Against Children” (NeMUP; www. nemup.de), which comprises five collaborative research sites from the field of (forensic) psychiatry or sexual medicine [...].
Recruitment was carried out by providing study advertisement/information in diverse internet platforms and some correctional services in Germany.

From the total NeMUP sample described elsewhere [Gerwinn et al., 2015b], fMRI data of a total of 117 male subjects matched with respect to age and IQ were analyzed.
Accordingly, 77 subjects met ICD-10 diagnostic criteria for pedophilia.

  • 37 pedophiles without a history of sexual offending against children (P-CSO) were recruited from the community (n=20) or the “Prevention Project Dunkelfeld” (PPD) [Beier et al., 2009] (n517).
  • 40 pedophiles with a history of sexual offenses against children (P+CSO) were taken from the community (n=16), and the PPD (n=9), and from correctional institutions (n=15).

[... ... ...]

A history of CSO was defined as the individual involvement in at least one case of sexual offenses against children under the age of 14, which includes actions of touching or manipulating the child’s naked body and/or genitals with the aim of sexually stimulating himself, penetrating the child anally/vaginally, or making the child touch or manipulate the offender’s genitals or penetrating him.

Exclusion criteria were neurological or acute psychiatric disorders other than pedophilia, acute episodes of alcohol or drug abuse/dependence, as well as past dependencies (past episodes of substance misuse were included), and current medication related to sexual functioning.

None of the participants took psychotropic medication other than antidepressants for a period of at least 3 weeks before assessment. Antidepressant medication was evident in

  • 1 P1+SO (SSNRI),
  • 2 P-CSO (SSRI, NDRI), and
  • 2 healthy controls (SSNRI, anticonvulsive).

The healthy control group recruited from the community throughout advertisements in public institutions included 40 men without a history of criminal behavior or current psychiatric disorders. [... ...]

Assessment

Data were acquired in two separate sessions distributed over 2 days. Initially (first session), the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM (SCID) [Wittchen et al., 1997] accompanied by a semi-structured clinical interview were completed to assess for DSM-IV-TR [American Psychiatric Association, 2000] Axis I and II disorders, as well as sexual age, gender, and interaction interest(s), child pornography consumption or offence history in general. Sexual gender and age orientation was confirmed by means of a modified version of the Kinsey scale for developmental stages [Kirk et al., 2000]. In cases of uncertainty regarding the sexual age orientation, a viewing-time paradigm, legal information (if available) and individual case conferences were utilized to ensure valid clinical diagnoses.

Global intelligence was estimated by means of 4 subtests (‘Similarities’, ‘Vocabulary’, ‘Block Design’, and ‘Matrix Reasoning’) derived from the German version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th Edition (WAIS-IV) [Von Aster et al., 2006]. All assessments were carried out through experienced research associates, trained to use these instruments. In a second session, the MRI assessment was performed.

Go/Nogo Task

As part of a more extensive MRI examination, participants performed on an event-related go/nogo task that was preceded by an alertness task comprised of go-trials only. The latter task was implemented to allow the participants to familiarize themselves with the MRI environment and to provide a baseline measure for reaction times in response to simple target stimuli. [...]

Initially, an instruction screen was presented which informed the participants to respond as fast as possible to any of the presented alertness stimuli indicated by the letter “X”. After reading the instructions, the stimulus presentation of 50 alertness trials was initiated throughout an individual response.

This condition was followed by a second instruction-screen introducing the go/nogo condition comprised of 150 trials. Participants were required to respond as fast as possible to the frequently (80% of trials) presented go-trials (indicated by the letter “X”) but were instructed to withhold their responses to the infrequently (20% of trials) occurring nogo-trials (indicated by a “+” sign). [... ...]

Prior to scanning, participants were instructed to respond with the right forefinger on the response box. To provide a behavioral measure of response inhibition, the amount of commission errors of nogo-trials, and reaction times (RTs) to alertness as well as to go-trials were used for further statistical analysis. Extreme-values that differed more than threefold from the distance between the 25% and 75% percentile of the distribution were excluded from further analysis. [...]

Imagibg Parameters and Processing

[... ... technical specification of the used devices ... ...]

Statistical Analysis

[... ... details of the used statistical methods ... ...]

fMRI Analysis

[... ... details of the used devices and the arrangement and processing of the data ... ....]

Results

Characteristics of Study Groups

As shown in Table 1, there were no significant differences between study groups with respect to age, sexual gender orientation, [left-]handedness [*], and WAIS IQ score.

Regarding prevalence of mental and personality disorders, there were no significant differences between both pedophilic groups (... ... ....) but both pedophilic groups yielded higher rates then did healthy controls.

Moreover, both pedophilic groups did not differ according to child pornography consumption (lifetime) and the ratio of exclusive/non-exclusive type of pedophilia. Details on clinical diagnoses are provided in Table 1. [*]

  • [* View the tables and figures in the added PDF version of the whole article - Ipce]

Within-group Pearsons correlations were calculated between age and WAIS IQ score and behavioral performance (reaction times to go-trials, commission errors, and errors to go-trials).

  • In the P+CSO group [... it appeared that] the higher the IQ estimates, the less errors to go-trials.
  • Similarly, in the P-CSO group WAIS IQ score was (marginally) negatively associated with errors to go-trials (...).
  • In healthy controls, [... it appeared that] the older the controls are, the longer their reaction times and the more errors were produced.[... and that] higher IQ estimates were related to accelerated reaction times.

There were no further correlations and none of the reported findings remained significant after Bonferroni correction. [*]

  • [* Not mentioned here, but still remarkably are some significant correllations mentioned in Table 1:
    • Depression and substance use were seen significantly frequenter in the P+CSO and the P-CSO group, than in the HC (Health Controls) group.
    • The Antisocial personality Disorder was only seen in the P+CSO groep, not in the P-CSO group and the HC group. - Ipce

Behavior Results

As depicted in Figure 1a,b, [* See the PDF version] there was a main effect of condition showing increased RTs [Reaction Time] in go as compared to alertness trials (...), but no significant group-by-condition interaction effect, indicating no between-group difference in any RT measure.

However, as shown in Figure 1c, error analyses revealed a significant main effect of group (...) indicating better inhibitory control in P-CSO as compared to P+CSO (... ...), while the difference between P-CSO and HC (...) as well as P+CSO and HC (...) failed to reach statistical significance. [....]

fMRI Results

Analysis of the fMRI data was performed using two different approaches:

  • a common approach were the eventrelated responses were convolved with the canonical HRF only and
  • another approach utilizing the canonical HRF plus the corresponding temporal derivative term to control for potential amplitude bias effects.

In the following, we report the statistics from the common SPM analysis and it is indicated when any significant differences between both approaches were obtained.

As expected, main effect of task (nogo > go condition) revealed changes in BOLD responses in bilateral frontoparietal areas including the

  • inferior parietal cortex,
  • middle frontal cortex,
  • PCC,
  • medial frontal cortex including the
    • ACC and
    • supplementary motor cortex,
  • middle temporal cortex,
  • anterior insula,
  • superior frontal cortex as well as
  • the DLPFC and
  • the left middle occipital cortex.

For a detailed overview see Table II and Figure 2 [in the PDF version].

There was a significant group-by-condition interaction effect (... ...). Using the temporal derivative approach, the peak voxel of this interaction effect moved (... ...)

As illustrated in Figure 3, post-hoc comparisons of the contrast P-CSO > P+CSO revealed increased activation in the left PCC (... ... ...) and left superior frontal cortex (SFC) (... ...) during response inhibition (nogo > go trials). Notably, the latter effect in the SFC did not remain significant using the temporal derivative approach.

No other between-group comparisons (... ... ...) revealed statistically significant findings. The same was true for the group comparisons regarding the contrasts commission errors vs. baseline and commission errors vs. correct omitted nogo trials. The same was true for the group comparisins regarding the contrasts commission errors vs. baseline and commission errpre vs. correct omotted nogo trials.

The same was true for the correlation analysis. We did not find any significant association between behavioral performance and BOLD responses, neither across groups nor within groups.

Discussion

This is the first study using event related fMRI in combination with a behavioral go/nogo paradigm, to analyze inhibitory control capacity and underlying inhibitionrelated neural activation pattern with respect to pedophilia and/or sexual offending against children.

As compared to offending pedophiles, non-offending pedophiles exhibited superior inhibitory control as reflected by a significantly lower rate of commission errors. In line with previous fMRI research [Blasi et al., 2006; Bari and Robbins, 2013; Simmonds et al., 2008], response inhibition led to stronger recruitment of a fronto-parietal control network  (FPCN).

Congruent with our hypothesis, offending pedophiles revealed decreased activation of the medial parietal cortex including the left caudal PCC as well as the left SFC as compared to non-offending pedophiles, but contrary to our hypotheses, showed no activation difference in prefrontal areas.

Also as expected, non-offending pedophiles and healthy controls did not differ with respect to any measure, and differences between offending pedophiles and healthy controls also did not reach significance.

Previous studies provided no clear evidence for the interpretation of increased impulsivity in pedophiles [Joyal et al., 2014]. More specifically, during the assessment of a letter based go/nogo task, increased frequencies of commission errors were documented in non-pedophilic but only marginally in pedophilic child sexual offenders [Schiffer and Vonlaufen, 2011], while Habermeyer et al. [2013b] reported augmented amount of commission errors in pedophilic offenders relative to controls.

Both former studies (and nearly all other studies in the field) included only
mixed groups
of offending pedophiles, thus precluding researchers from assessing whether differences relate to sexual preference, i.e., pedophilia or sexual deviant behavior, i.e., CSO.

In contrast, in the current study, we had the very scarce opportunity to distinguish between offending and non-offending pedophiles, which allowed for a more in depth analysis of the question, whether impulsivity relates to CSO rather than pedophilia. In the present study, nonoffending as compared to offending pedophiles revealed significantly decreased commission errors but both pedophilic groups did not differ from healthy controls. Accordingly, our results point to the interpretation of more elaborated self-control abilities in pedophiles who did not act on their sexual preference, rather than a more impulsive response style in offending pedophiles.

The absence of executive abnormalities in the present group of offending pedophiles is in contrast to other studies [Suchy et al., 2009, 2014]. An explanation for this inconsistency might be related to the status of incarceration, which is likely to be associated with diverse deprivation effects [Lapornik et al., 1996] and could at least partly be responsible for executive deficiencies in previous (incarcerated) samples of offending pedophiles.

However, in contrast to almost all previous studies, only every fourth offender in the current study was incarcerated at the time of assessment, while the majority of them were recruited from the community or the PPD, respectively. None of the aforementioned studies controlled for the effect of incarceration, so we assume that our more naturalistic and therefore less confounded sample reflect a more realistic picture of executive functioning in pedophilic offenders than did previous studies.

Although the PCC has not been reported consistently in the context of response inhibition, previous fMRI studies demonstrated aberrant PCC activity in clinical populations showing inhibitory control deficits, as for instance in ADHD [Rubia et al., 2005] or Williams Syndrome [Mobbs et al., 2007].

Steele and colleagues [2014], who assessed a go/nogo task in a large sample of healthy participants, proposed a rather indirect association between the PCC and response inhibition, since this region has been related to consciousness, episodic memory, and self-referential mental processes and therefore was thought to play an important role in the interplay of various neural networks [Leech and Sharp, 2014].

The PCC has been reported as a key structure of the DMN, showing highly correlated activity during rest and reactive deactivation when attention is directed externally [Gusnard et al., 2001; Raichle et al., 2001]. The magnitude of hemodynamic responses in the PCC have been found to be associated with cognitive load in the healthy brain, while the inability of appropriate deactivation is related to disturbed cognitive function in the healthy as well as in the damaged brain [Leech and Sharp, 2014].
Moreover, aberrant PCC responses were reported in pedophile offenders during rest [Kärgel et al., 2015] as well as during the processing of preferred sexual stimuli [Poeppl et al., 2011].

Our finding of inhibition-related PCC activation in a pedophilic sub-sample is largely in line with previous findings by Habermeyer et al. [2013b], who proposed an attenuated deactivation of the DMN in pedophiles during the cognitive demanding nogo condition. Consequently, they suggested that pedophiles might be involved in selfreferential mental activities more than in applying taskrelated cognitive control. However, since the authors assessed a mixed group of offending pedophiles, it is not possible to conclude from this study whether this activation pattern relates to pedophilia or CSO.

In contrast to the DMN [= Default Mode Network], enhanced BOLD activity [= a specific pattern] has been observed in the FPCN during cognitive demanding tasks where attention is directed to external information [Leech and Sharp, 2014]. It is suggested that neural responses of both networks are tightly coupled and exhibit an anti-correlated activity/connectivity pattern [Kelly et al., 2008; Leech and Sharp, 2014].

In the present study no differential PCC activity was observed between both conditions (nogo > go) in healthy controls. Notably, during the cognitively more demanding nogo-trials offending pedophiles exhibited decreased activity in the PCC as compared to non-offending pedophiles, whereas an opposite response pattern was found during the processing of go-trials.

Following the idea that the PCC plays a central role in linking the abovementioned neural networks thus allowing for efficient cognitive function, our results might indicate dysfunctional or diminished engagement of the FPCN in pedophilic offenders during cognitive demanding tasks on the one hand and inappropriately increased FPCN involvement during the less challenging cognitive tasks on the other hand.

By contrast, pedophiles who did not engage in CSO seem to show a neural mechanism with probably compensatory function. This mechanism that also utilizes parts of the PCC, results in an appropriate inhibition-related activation of the FPCN (i.e., during the nogo condition).

This notion is strongly supported by the fact that the left SFC, which is also part of the FPCN [Niendam et al., 2012] showed a similar response pattern. The functional significance of the SFC is thought to be related to executive functioning, particularly to working memory function [du Boisgueheneuc et al., 2006; Rowe et al., 2000]. This domain, which comprises an essential aspect of response inhibition allows an individual to appropriately maintain internal representations of task goals.

The hypothesis that “compensatory” engagement of the FPCN might help preventing pedophiles from sexual offending against children is quite speculative, but warrants further investigation. Interestingly, our findings of neurofunctional group differences in neural structures essential for efficient cognitive functioning may support the idea of a general processing deficit in offending pedophiles as proposed by Suchy et al. [2014], which, however, seems to be related to sexual offending behavior rather than aberrant sexual interests in general.

Disinhibition has been discussed as a predictor of sexual offending within diverse theories, i.e., the self-regulation model [Ward et al., 1998] or the motivation-facilitation model [Seto, 2008]. It is suggested that only individuals who can overcome their inhibitions, e.g., worries about potential negative consequences on the victim, or fear to violate against social norms, would show an increased  propensity to sexually approach victims.

Although disinhibition has been reported a relevant risk factor for sexual recidivism in general [Hanson and Morton-Bourgon, 2005; Mann et al., 2010], in line with previous research on personality traits [Cohen and Galynker, 2012] and cognitive functioning in pedophilia [Schiffer and Vonlaufen, 2011], our findings indicate that response inhibition abilities are not reduced in pedophilia per se.

The present study rather extends the existing literature by providing some evidence that in pedophilia the ability not to offend against children might be associated with an elevated level of inhibitory control as well as an increased inhibition-related recruitment of the FPCN.

Strengths and limitations

Inconsistent localization of stimulus projection screen and unbalanced sample distribution across fMRI scanners may contribute to a potential site confound of our findings. Thus, a control analysis was conducted including N=87 controls (... in the different locations ...) to compare them with regard to percent signal change (PSC) values extracted from relevant structures associated with response inhibition.

Our results demonstrate that PSC values taken from specific ROIs including the ACC, bilateral Insula, and inferior parietal cortex do not systematically differ across sites. Accordingly, and moreover due to the observation that inclusion vs. exclusion of site as a covariate did not significantly influence the present results, a site confound is rather unlikely to account for the findings at hand.

The recruitment of pedophiles with a history of sexual offending against children from community and the PPD represented a principle strength of the present investigation, however, to conclusively disentangle the response inhibition related correlates of pedophilia from those associated with sexual offending against children, the inclusion of a non-pedophilic CSO group would have been an
important addition.

Another limitation was inherent to the proportions of participants suffering from (other) mental or personality disorders. Both pedophilic groups showed higher rates of both mental and personality disorders than did healthy controls. Nevertheless, since the effects that we reported here were restricted to differences between both
pedophilic groups, and both pedophilic groups were similar with respect to number of patients suffering from other mental or personality disorders, this limitation could not have affected the findings discussed here.

Conclusions

Taken together, our results suggest that

  • (1) pedophiles without a history of CSO may have increased self-control abilities,
  • (2) pedophiles in general are not likely to be more impulsive than controls, and
  • (3) pedophiles who did not commit hands-on offenses against children might deploy a neural compensatory mechanism involving parts of the FPCN facilitating efficient response inhibition or cognitive control in general.

Our present fMRI study, of course, cannot fully explain the discrepant findings of previous investigations assessing for the neurobiological correlates of pedophilia and CSO but, nonetheless, might provide essential methodological indications reflecting the importance to separate pedophilic hands-on offenders from those that have not sexually offended against children.

The finding that groups differed more according to their deviant sexual behavior than according to their sexual preference pattern may represent an important shift in how these groups are considered. From a clinical point of view, the present data thus indicate that intervention strategies aimed at fostering basic inhibitory control abilities might be useful for preventing CSO in both pedophiles who already engaged in CSO as well as those at risk.